癢這種感覺為什麼會存在?

本人理解是人的感覺一般是對機體自身的反應,結果是促使機體去做有益的改變。
比如疼痛。因為碰到燙的東西感到疼痛,所以人會遠離。疼痛導致的是避免或者使自己不再收到傷害。可是癢呢?比如傷口癢,我知道是神經末梢在生長的原因,可是癢導致的結果不是撓嗎?可是這個結果顯然不是應該做的,撓破傷口對於身體不是有益的啊,為什麼會有這種「多餘」的感覺?
相關問題癢是一種痛覺嗎?


大約10年前人們還把癢作為一種微弱的痛覺來看待,換句話說,相當於一種在痛覺神經中傳遞的微弱信號,介於有接觸和有痛覺之間。近年來的一系列研究逐漸認為,癢和痛覺有各自的獨立傳導途徑,在對癢的研究上也有了突破性進展。但由於研究的時間還很短,癢的機制和進化上有什麼意義現在還是很難說清楚。

鑒於癢和痛的相似性:都是迫使人作出某種行為的不愉快感覺,多數接受信息的感受器位於皮膚,有相似的傳導通路等,我們可以推測,和痛覺一樣,癢也有讓我們遠離危險的作用,只不過痛覺的反應是避開,而癢的反應是抓撓。最初人類抓撓的目的可能是去除一些皮膚上的昆蟲和植物毒素,經過不斷的演化保留了下來。當然,每個人都有感受,大多數情況下,抓撓確實有止癢的作用。而對有的人來說,抓撓引起的皮膚炎症會讓皮膚更癢;對一些皮膚病如過敏性皮炎,或者系統性疾病或神經病變的患者來說,抓撓又起不了什麼作用。這背後的機制和應對方法,還需要更多的研究來探索。

如果對這個話題感興趣,08年The New Yorker有過一篇不錯的科普文章:http://www.newyorker.com/reporting/2008/06/30/080630fa_fact_gawande?currentPage=all,松鼠會有過翻譯:科學松鼠會, @葉一 也在癢是一種痛覺嗎?這個題目下貼出來了。這是一篇有趣的文章,其中睡著的時候抓破顱骨的案例更是觸目驚心和印象深刻。


癢可不簡單,有專門的神經負責的。轉一篇經典長文,供參考(抓癢抓破頭蓋骨):

Scratching an itch through the scalp to the brain : The New Yorker

THE ITCH
Its mysterious power may be a clue to a new theory about brains and bodies.BY ATUL GAWANDE

It was still shocking to M. how much a few wrong turns could change your life. She had graduated from Boston College with a degree in psychology, married at twenty-five, and had two children, a son and a daughter. She and her family settled in a town on Massachusetts』 southern shore. She worked for thirteen years in health care, becoming the director of a residence program for men who』d suffered severe head injuries. But she and her husband began fighting. There were betrayals. By the time she was thirty-two, her marriage had disintegrated. In the divorce, she lost possession of their home, and, amid her financial and psychological struggles, she saw that she was losing her children, too. Within a few years, she was drinking. She began dating someone, and they drank together. After a while, he brought some drugs home, and she tried them. The drugs got harder. Eventually, they were doing heroin, which turned out to be readily available from a street dealer a block away from her apartment.

One day, she went to see a doctor because she wasn』t feeling well, and learned that she had contracted H.I.V. from a contaminated needle. She had to leave her job. She lost visiting rights with her children. And she developed complications from the H.I.V., including shingles, which caused painful, blistering sores across her scalp and forehead. With treatment, though, her H.I.V. was brought under control. At thirty-six, she entered rehab, dropped the boyfriend, and kicked the drugs. She had two good, quiet years in which she began rebuilding her life. Then she got the itch.

It was right after a shingles episode. The blisters and the pain responded, as they usually did, to acyclovir, an antiviral medication. But this time the area of the scalp that was involved became numb, and the pain was replaced by a constant, relentless itch. She felt it mainly on the right side of her head. It crawled along her scalp, and no matter how much she scratched it would not go away. 「I felt like my inner self, like my brain itself, was itching,」 she says. And it took over her life just as she was starting to get it back.

Her internist didn』t know what to make of the problem. Itching is an extraordinarily common symptom. All kinds of dermatological conditions can cause it: allergic reactions, bacterial or fungal infections, skin cancer, psoriasis, dandruff, scabies, lice, poison ivy, sun damage, or just dry skin. Creams and makeup can cause itch, too. But M. used ordinary shampoo and soap, no creams. And when the doctor examined M.』s scalp she discovered nothing abnormal—no rash, no redness, no scaling, no thickening, no fungus, no parasites. All she saw was scratch marks.

The internist prescribed a medicated cream, but it didn』t help. The urge to scratch was unceasing and irresistible. 「I would try to control it during the day, when I was aware of the itch, but it was really hard,」 M. said. 「At night, it was the worst. I guess I would scratch when I was asleep, because in the morning there would be blood on my pillowcase.」 She began to lose her hair over the itchy area. She returned to her internist again and again. 「I just kept haunting her and calling her,」 M. said. But nothing the internist tried worked, and she began to suspect that the itch had nothing to do with M.』s skin.

Plenty of non-skin conditions can cause itching. Dr. Jeffrey Bernhard, a dermatologist with the University of Massachusetts Medical School, is among the few doctors to study itching systematically (he published the definitive textbook on the subject), and he told me of cases caused by hyperthyroidism, iron deficiency, liver disease, and cancers like Hodgkin』s lymphoma. Sometimes the syndrome is very specific. Persistent outer-arm itching that worsens in sunlight is known asbrachioradial pruritus, and it』s caused by a crimped nerve in the neck.Aquagenic pruritus is recurrent, intense, diffuse itching upon getting out of a bath or shower, and although no one knows the mechanism, it』s a symptom of polycythemia vera, a rare condition in which the body produces too many red blood cells.

But M.』s itch was confined to the right side of her scalp. Her viral count showed that the H.I.V. was quiescent. Additional blood tests and X-rays were normal. So the internist concluded that M.』s problem was probably psychiatric. All sorts of psychiatric conditions can cause itching. Patients with psychosis can have cutaneous delusions—a belief that their skin is infested with, say, parasites, or crawling ants, or laced with tiny bits of fibreglass. Severe stress and other emotional experiences can also give rise to a physical symptom like itching—whether from the body』s release of endorphins (natural opioids, which, like morphine, can cause itching), increased skin temperature, nervous scratching, or increased sweating. In M.』s case, the internist suspected tricho-tillomania, an obsessive-compulsive disorder in which patients have an irresistible urge to pull out their hair.

M. was willing to consider such possibilities. Her life had been a mess, after all. But the antidepressant medications often prescribed for O.C.D. made no difference. And she didn』t actually feel a compulsion to pull out her hair. She simply felt itchy, on the area of her scalp that was left numb from the shingles. Although she could sometimes distract herself from it—by watching television or talking with a friend—the itch did not fluctuate with her mood or level of stress. The only thing that came close to offering relief was to scratch.

「Scratching is one of the sweetest gratifications of nature, and as ready at hand as any,」 Montaigne wrote. 「But repentance follows too annoyingly close at its heels.」 For M., certainly, it did: the itching was so torturous, and the area so numb, that her scratching began to go through the skin. At a later office visit, her doctor found a silver-dollar-size patch of scalp where skin had been replaced by scab. M. tried bandaging her head, wearing caps to bed. But her fingernails would always find a way to her flesh, especially while she slept.

One morning, after she was awakened by her bedside alarm, she sat up and, she recalled, 「this fluid came down my face, this greenish liquid.」 She pressed a square of gauze to her head and went to see her doctor again. M. showed the doctor the fluid on the dressing. The doctor looked closely at the wound. She shined a light on it and in M.』s eyes. Then she walked out of the room and called an ambulance. Only in the Emergency Department at Massachusetts General Hospital, after the doctors started swarming, and one told her she needed surgerynow, did M. learn what had happened. She had scratched through her skull during the night—and all the way into her brain.

Itching is a most peculiar and diabolical sensation. The definition offered by the German physician Samuel Hafenreffer in 1660 has yet to be improved upon: An unpleasant sensation that provokes the desire to scratch. Itch has been ranked, by scientific and artistic observers alike, among the most distressing physical sensations one can experience. In Dante』s Inferno, falsifiers were punished by 「the burning rage / of fierce itching that nothing could relieve」:

The way their nails scraped down upon the
scabs
Was like a knife scraping off scales from
carp. . . .
「O you there tearing at your mail of
scabs
And even turning your fingers into
pincers,」
My guide began addressing one of them,

「Tell us are there Italians among the
souls
Down in this hole and I』ll pray that your
nails
Will last you in this task eternally.」

Though scratching can provide momentary relief, it often makes the itching worse. Dermatologists call this the itch-scratch cycle. Scientists believe that itch, and the accompanying scratch reflex, evolved in order to protect us from insects and clinging plant toxins—from such dangers as malaria, yellow fever, and dengue, transmitted by mosquitoes; from tularemia, river blindness, and sleeping sickness, transmitted by flies; from typhus-bearing lice, plague-bearing fleas, and poisonous spiders. The theory goes a long way toward explaining why itch is so exquisitely tuned. You can spend all day without noticing the feel of your shirt collar on your neck, and yet a single stray thread poking out, or a louse』s fine legs brushing by, can set you scratching furiously.

But how, exactly, itch works has been a puzzle. For most of medical history, scientists thought that itching was merely a weak form of pain. Then, in 1987, the German researcher H. O. Handwerker and his colleagues used mild electric pulses to drive histamine, an itch-producing substance that the body releases during allergic reactions, into the skin of volunteers. As the researchers increased the dose of histamine, they found that they were able to increase the intensity of itch the volunteers reported, from the barely appreciable to the 「maximum imaginable.」 Yet the volunteers never felt an increase in pain. The scientists concluded that itch and pain are entirely separate sensations, transmitted along different pathways.

Despite centuries spent mapping the body』s nervous circuitry, scientists had never noticed a nerve specific for itch. But now the hunt was on, and a group of Swedish and German researchers embarked upon a series of tricky experiments. They inserted ultra-thin metal electrodes into the skin of paid volunteers, and wiggled them around until they picked up electrical signals from a single nerve fibre. Computers subtracted the noise from other nerve fibres crossing through the region. The researchers would then spend hours—as long as the volunteer could tolerate it—testing different stimuli on the skin in the area (a heated probe, for example, or a fine paintbrush) to see what would get the nerve to fire, and what the person experienced when it did.

They worked their way through fifty-three volunteers. Mostly, they encountered well-known types of nerve fibres that respond to temperature or light touch or mechanical pressure. 「That feels warm,」 a volunteer might say, or 「That feels soft,」 or 「Ouch! Hey!」 Several times, the scientists came across a nerve fibre that didn』t respond to any of these stimuli. When they introduced a tiny dose of histamine into the skin, however, they observed a sharp electrical response in some of these nerve fibres, and the volunteer would experience an itch. They announced their discovery in a 1997 paper: they』d found a type of nerve that was specific for itch.

Unlike, say, the nerve fibres for pain, each of which covers a millimetre-size territory, a single itch fibre can pick up an itchy sensation more than three inches away. The fibres also turned out to have extraordinarily low conduction speeds, which explained why itchiness is so slow to build and so slow to subside.

Other researchers traced these fibres to the spinal cord and all the way to the brain. Examining functional pet-scan studies in healthy human subjects who had been given mosquito-bite-like histamine injections, they found a distinct signature of itch activity. Several specific areas of the brain light up: the part of the cortex that tells you where on your body the sensation occurs; the region that governs your emotional responses, reflecting the disagreeable nature of itch; and the limbic and motor areas that process irresistible urges (such as the urge to use drugs, among the addicted, or to overeat, among the obese), reflecting the ferocious impulse to scratch.

Now various phenomena became clear. Itch, it turns out, is indeed inseparable from the desire to scratch. It can be triggered chemically (by the saliva injected when a mosquito bites, say) or mechanically (from the mosquito』s legs, even before it bites). The itch-scratch reflex activates higher levels of your brain than the spinal-cord-level reflex that makes you pull your hand away from a flame. Brain scans also show that scratching diminishes activity in brain areas associated with unpleasant sensations.

But some basic features of itch remained unexplained—features that make itch a uniquely revealing case study. On the one hand, our bodies are studded with receptors for itch, as they are with receptors for touch, pain, and other sensations; this provides an alarm system for harm and allows us to safely navigate the world. But why does a feather brushed across the skin sometimes itch and at other times tickle? (Tickling has a social component: you can make yourself itch, but only another person can tickle you.) And, even more puzzling, how is it that you can make yourself itchy just by thinking about it?

Contemplating what it』s like to hold your finger in a flame won』t make your finger hurt. But simply writing about a tick crawling up the nape of one』s neck is enough to start my neck itching. Then my scalp. And then this one little spot along my flank where I』m beginning to wonder whether I should check to see if there might be something there. In one study, a German professor of psychosomatics gave a lecture that included, in the first half, a series of what might be called itchy slides, showing fleas, lice, people scratching, and the like, and, in the second half, more benign slides, with pictures of soft down, baby skin, bathers. Video cameras recorded the audience. Sure enough, the frequency of scratching among people in the audience increased markedly during the first half and decreased during the second. Thoughts made them itch.

We now have the nerve map for itching, as we do for other sensations. But a deeper puzzle remains: how much of our sensations and experiences do nerves really explain?

In the operating room, a neurosurgeon washed out and debrided M.』s wound, which had become infected. Later, a plastic surgeon covered it with a graft of skin from her thigh. Though her head was wrapped in layers of gauze and she did all she could to resist the still furious itchiness, she awoke one morning to find that she had rubbed the graft away. The doctors returned her to the operating room for a second skin graft, and this time they wrapped her hands as well. She rubbed it away again anyway.

「They kept telling me I had O.C.D.,」 M. said. A psychiatric team was sent in to see her each day, and the resident would ask her, 「As a child, when you walked down the street did you count the lines? Did you do anything repetitive? Did you have to count everything you saw?」 She kept telling him no, but he seemed skeptical. He tracked down her family and asked them, but they said no, too. Psychology tests likewise ruled out obsessive-compulsive disorder. They showed depression, though, and, of course, there was the history of addiction. So the doctors still thought her scratching was from a psychiatric disorder. They gave her drugs that made her feel logy and sleep a lot. But the itching was as bad as ever, and she still woke up scratching at that terrible wound.

One morning, she found, as she put it, 「this very bright and happy-looking woman standing by my bed. She said, 『I』m Dr. Oaklander,』 」 M. recalled. 「I thought, Oh great. Here we go again. But she explained that she was a neurologist, and she said, 『The first thing I want to say to you is that I don』t think you』re crazy. I don』t think you have O.C.D.』 At that moment, I really saw her grow wings and a halo,」 M. told me. 「I said, 『Are you sure?』 And she said, 『Yes. I have heard of this before.』 」

Anne Louise Oaklander was about the same age as M. Her mother is a prominent neurologist at Albert Einstein College of Medicine, in New York, and she』d followed her into the field. Oaklander had specialized in disorders of peripheral nerve sensation—disorders like shingles. Although pain is the most common symptom of shingles, Oaklander had noticed during her training that some patients also had itching, occasionally severe, and seeing M. reminded her of one of her shingles patients. 「I remember standing in a hallway talking to her, and what she complained about—her major concern—was that she was tormented by this terrible itch over the eye where she had had shingles,」 she told me. When Oaklander looked at her, she thought that something wasn』t right. It took a moment to realize why. 「The itch was so severe, she had scratched off her eyebrow.」

Oaklander tested the skin near M.』s wound. It was numb to temperature, touch, and pinprick. Nonetheless, it was itchy, and when it was scratched or rubbed M. felt the itchiness temporarily subside. Oaklander injected a few drops of local anesthetic into the skin. To M.』s surprise, the itching stopped—instantly and almost entirely. This was the first real relief she』d had in more than a year.

It was an imperfect treatment, though. The itch came back when the anesthetic wore off, and, although Oaklander tried having M. wear an anesthetic patch over the wound, the effect diminished over time. Oaklander did not have an explanation for any of this. When she took a biopsy of the itchy skin, it showed that ninety-six per cent of the nerve fibres were gone. So why was the itch so intense?

Oaklander came up with two theories. The first was that those few remaining nerve fibres were itch fibres and, with no other fibres around to offer competing signals, they had become constantly active. The second theory was the opposite. The nerves were dead, but perhaps the itch system in M.』s brain had gone haywire, running on a loop all its own.

The second theory seemed less likely. If the nerves to her scalp were dead, how would you explain the relief she got from scratching, or from the local anesthetic? Indeed, how could you explain the itch in the first place? An itch without nerve endings didn』t make sense. The neurosurgeons stuck with the first theory; they offered to cut the main sensory nerve to the front of M.』s scalp and abolish the itching permanently. Oaklander, however, thought that the second theory was the right one—that this was a brain problem, not a nerve problem—and that cutting the nerve would do more harm than good. She argued with the neurosurgeons, and she advised M. not to let them do any cutting.

「But I was desperate,」 M. told me. She let them operate on her, slicing the supraorbital nerve above the right eye. When she woke up, a whole section of her forehead was numb—and the itching was gone. A few weeks later, however, it came back, in an even wider expanse than before. The doctors tried pain medications, more psychiatric medications, more local anesthetic. But the only thing that kept M. from tearing her skin and skull open again, the doctors found, was to put a foam football helmet on her head and bind her wrists to the bedrails at night.

She spent the next two years committed to a locked medical ward in a rehabilitation hospital—because, although she was not mentally ill, she was considered a danger to herself. Eventually, the staff worked out a solution that did not require binding her to the bedrails. Along with the football helmet, she had to wear white mitts that were secured around her wrists by surgical tape. 「Every bedtime, it looked like they were dressing me up for Halloween—me and the guy next to me,」 she told me.

「The guy next to you?」 I asked. He had had shingles on his neck, she explained, and also developed a persistent itch. 「Every night, they would wrap up his hands and wrap up mine.」 She spoke more softly now. 「But I heard he ended up dying from it, because he scratched into his carotid artery.」

I met M. seven years after she』d been discharged from the rehabilitation hospital. She is forty-eight now. She lives in a three-room apartment, with a crucifix and a bust of Jesus on the wall and the low yellow light of table lamps strung with beads over their shades. Stacked in a wicker basket next to her coffee table were Rick Warren』s 「The Purpose Driven Life,」 People, and the latest issue ofNeurology Now, a magazine for patients. Together, they summed up her struggles, for she is still fighting the meaninglessness, the isolation, and the physiology of her predicament.

She met me at the door in a wheelchair; the injury to her brain had left her partially paralyzed on the left side of her body. She remains estranged from her children. She has not, however, relapsed into drinking or drugs. Her H.I.V. remains under control. Although the itch on her scalp and forehead persists, she has gradually learned to protect herself. She trims her nails short. She finds ways to distract herself. If she must scratch, she tries to rub gently instead. And, if that isn』t enough, she uses a soft toothbrush or a rolled-up terry cloth. 「I don』t use anything sharp,」 she said. The two years that she spent bound up in the hospital seemed to have broken the nighttime scratching. At home, she found that she didn』t need to wear the helmet and gloves anymore.

Still, the itching remains a daily torment. 「I don』t normally tell people this,」 she said, 「but I have a fantasy of shaving off my eyebrow and taking a metal-wire grill brush and scratching away.」

Some of her doctors have not been willing to let go of the idea that this has been a nerve problem all along. A local neurosurgeon told her that the original operation to cut the sensory nerve to her scalp must not have gone deep enough. 「He wants to go in again,」 she told me.

A new scientific understanding of perception has emerged in the past few decades, and it has overturned classical, centuries-long beliefs about how our brains work—though it has apparently not penetrated the medical world yet. The old understanding of perception is what neuroscientists call 「the na?ve view,」 and it is the view that most people, in or out of medicine, still have. We』re inclined to think that people normally perceive things in the world directly. We believe that the hardness of a rock, the coldness of an ice cube, the itchiness of a sweater are picked up by our nerve endings, transmitted through the spinal cord like a message through a wire, and decoded by the brain.

In a 1710 「Treatise Concerning the Principles of Human Knowledge,」 the Irish philosopher George Berkeley objected to this view. We do not know the world of objects, he argued; we know only our mental ideas of objects. 「Light and colours, heat and cold, extension and figures—in a word, the things we see and feel—what are they but so many sensations, notions, ideas?」 Indeed, he concluded, the objects of the world are likely just inventions of the mind, put in there by God. To which Samuel Johnson famously responded by kicking a large stone and declaring, 「I refute it thus!

Still, Berkeley had recognized some serious flaws in the direct-perception theory—in the notion that when we see, hear, or feel we are just taking in the sights, sounds, and textures of the world. For one thing, it cannot explain how we experience things that seem physically real but aren』t: sensations of itching that arise from nothing more than itchy thoughts; dreams that can seem indistinguishable from reality; phantom sensations that amputees have in their missing limbs. And, the more we examine the actual nerve transmissions we receive from the world outside, the more inadequate they seem.

Our assumption had been that the sensory data we receive from our eyes, ears, nose, fingers, and so on contain all the information that we need for perception, and that perception must work something like a radio. It』s hard to conceive that a Boston Symphony Orchestra concert is in a radio wave. But it is. So you might think that it』s the same with the signals we receive—that if you hooked up someone』s nerves to a monitor you could watch what the person is experiencing as if it were a television show.

Yet, as scientists set about analyzing the signals, they found them to be radically impoverished. Suppose someone is viewing a tree in a clearing. Given simply the transmissions along the optic nerve from the light entering the eye, one would not be able to reconstruct the three-dimensionality, or the distance, or the detail of the bark—attributes that we perceive instantly.

Or consider what neuroscientists call 「the binding problem.」 Tracking a dog as it runs behind a picket fence, all that your eyes receive is separated vertical images of the dog, with large slices missing. Yet somehow you perceive the mutt to be whole, an intact entity travelling through space. Put two dogs together behind the fence and you don』t think they』ve morphed into one. Your mind now configures the slices as two independent creatures.

The images in our mind are extraordinarily rich. We can tell if something is liquid or solid, heavy or light, dead or alive. But the information we work from is poor—a distorted, two-dimensional transmission with entire spots missing. So the mind fills in most of the picture. You can get a sense of this from brain-anatomy studies. If visual sensations were primarily received rather than constructed by the brain, you』d expect that most of the fibres going to the brain』s primary visual cortex would come from the retina. Instead, scientists have found that only twenty per cent do; eighty per cent come downward from regions of the brain governing functions like memory. Richard Gregory, a prominent British neuropsychologist, estimates that visual perception is more than ninety per cent memory and less than ten per cent sensory nerve signals. When Oaklander theorized that M.』s itch was endogenous, rather than generated by peripheral nerve signals, she was onto something important.

The fallacy of reducing perception to reception is especially clear when it comes to phantom limbs. Doctors have often explained such sensations as a matter of inflamed or frayed nerve endings in the stump sending aberrant signals to the brain. But this explanation should long ago have been suspect. Efforts by surgeons to cut back on the nerve typically produce the same results that M. had when they cut the sensory nerve to her forehead: a brief period of relief followed by a return of the sensation.

Moreover, the feelings people experience in their phantom limbs are far too varied and rich to be explained by the random firings of a bruised nerve. People report not just pain but also sensations of sweatiness, heat, texture, and movement in a missing limb. There is no experience people have with real limbs that they do not experience with phantom limbs. They feel their phantom leg swinging, water trickling down a phantom arm, a phantom ring becoming too tight for a phantom digit. Children have used phantom fingers to count and solve arithmetic problems. V. S. Ramachandran, an eminent neuroscientist at the University of California, San Diego, has written up the case of a woman who was born with only stumps at her shoulders, and yet, as far back as she could remember, felt herself to have arms and hands; she even feels herself gesticulating when she speaks. And phantoms do not occur just in limbs. Around half of women who have undergone a mastectomy experience a phantom breast, with the nipple being the most vivid part. You』ve likely had an experience of phantom sensation yourself. When the dentist gives you a local anesthetic, and your lip goes numb, the nerves go dead. Yet you don』t feel your lip disappear. Quite the opposite: it feels larger and plumper than normal, even though you can see in a mirror that the size hasn』t changed.

The account of perception that』s starting to emerge is what we might call the 「brain』s best guess」 theory of perception: perception is the brain』s best guess about what is happening in the outside world. The mind integrates scattered, weak, rudimentary signals from a variety of sensory channels, information from past experiences, and hard-wired processes, and produces a sensory experience full of brain-provided color, sound, texture, and meaning. We see a friendly yellow Labrador bounding behind a picket fence not because that is the transmission we receive but because this is the perception our weaver-brain assembles as its best hypothesis of what is out there from the slivers of information we get. Perception is inference.

The theory—and a theory is all it is right now—has begun to make sense of some bewildering phenomena. Among them is an experiment that Ramachandran performed with volunteers who had phantom pain in an amputated arm. They put their surviving arm through a hole in the side of a box with a mirror inside, so that, peering through the open top, they would see their arm and its mirror image, as if they had two arms. Ramachandran then asked them to move both their intact arm and, in their mind, their phantom arm—to pretend that they were conducting an orchestra, say. The patients had the sense that they had two arms again. Even though they knew it was an illusion, it provided immediate relief. People who for years had been unable to unclench their phantom fist suddenly felt their hand open; phantom arms in painfully contorted positions could relax. With daily use of the mirror box over weeks, patients sensed their phantom limbs actually shrink into their stumps and, in several instances, completely vanish. Researchers at Walter Reed Army Medical Center recently published the results of a randomized trial of mirror therapy for soldiers with phantom-limb pain, showing dramatic success.

A lot about this phenomenon remains murky, but here』s what the new theory suggests is going on: when your arm is amputated, nerve transmissions are shut off, and the brain』s best guess often seems to be that the arm is still there, but paralyzed, or clenched, or beginning to cramp up. Things can stay like this for years. The mirror box, however, provides the brain with new visual input—however illusory—suggesting motion in the absent arm. The brain has to incorporate the new information into its sensory map of what』s happening. Therefore, it guesses again, and the pain goes away.

The new theory may also explain what was going on with M.』s itch. The shingles destroyed most of the nerves in her scalp. And, for whatever reason, her brain surmised from what little input it had that something horribly itchy was going on—that perhaps a whole army of ants were crawling back and forth over just that patch of skin. There wasn』t any such thing, of course. But M.』s brain has received no contrary signals that would shift its assumptions. So she itches.

Not long ago, I met a man who made me wonder whether such phantom sensations are more common than we realize. H. was forty-eight, in good health, an officer at a Boston financial-services company living with his wife in a western suburb, when he made passing mention of an odd pain to his internist. For at least twenty years, he said, he』d had a mild tingling running along his left arm and down the left side of his body, and, if he tilted his neck forward at a particular angle, it became a pronounced, electrical jolt. The internist recognized this as Lhermitte』s sign, a classic symptom that can indicate multiple sclerosis, Vitamin B12 deficiency, or spinal-cord compression from a tumor or a herniated disk. An MRI revealed a cavernous hemangioma, a pea-size mass of dilated blood vessels, pressing into the spinal cord in his neck. A week later, while the doctors were still contemplating what to do, it ruptured.

「I was raking leaves out in the yard and, all of a sudden, there was an explosion of pain and my left arm wasn』t responding to my brain,」 H. said when I visited him at home. Once the swelling subsided, a neurosurgeon performed a tricky operation to remove the tumor from the spinal cord. The operation was successful, but afterward H. began experiencing a constellation of strange sensations. His left hand felt cartoonishly large—at least twice its actual size. He developed a constant burning pain along an inch-wide ribbon extending from the left side of his neck all the way down his arm. And an itch crept up and down along the same band, which no amount of scratching would relieve.

H. has not accepted that these sensations are here to stay—the prospect is too depressing—but they』ve persisted for eleven years now. Although the burning is often tolerable during the day, the slightest thing can trigger an excruciating flareup—a cool breeze across the skin, the brush of a shirtsleeve or a bedsheet. 「Sometimes I feel that my skin has been flayed and my flesh is exposed, and any touch is just very painful,」 he told me. 「Sometimes I feel that there』s an ice pick or a wasp sting. Sometimes I feel that I』ve been splattered with hot cooking oil.」

For all that, the itch has been harder to endure. H. has developed calluses from the incessant scratching. 「I find I am choosing itch relief over the pain that I am provoking by satisfying the itch,」 he said.

He has tried all sorts of treatments—medications, acupuncture, herbal remedies, lidocaine injections, electrical-stimulation therapy. But nothing really worked, and the condition forced him to retire in 2001. He now avoids leaving the house. He gives himself projects. Last year, he built a three-foot stone wall around his yard, slowly placing the stones by hand. But he spends much of his day, after his wife has left for work, alone in the house with their three cats, his shirt off and the heat turned up, trying to prevent a flareup.

His neurologist introduced him to me, with his permission, as an example of someone with severe itching from a central rather than a peripheral cause. So one morning we sat in his living room trying to puzzle out what was going on. The sun streamed in through a big bay window. One of his cats, a scraggly brown tabby, curled up beside me on the couch. H. sat in an armchair in a baggy purple T-shirt he』d put on for my visit. He told me that he thought his problem was basically a 「bad switch」 in his neck where the tumor had been, a kind of loose wire sending false signals to his brain. But I told him about the increasing evidence that our sensory experiences are not sent to the brain but originate in it. When I got to the example of phantom-limb sensations, he perked up. The experiences of phantom-limb patients sounded familiar to him. When I mentioned that he might want to try the mirror-box treatment, he agreed. 「I have a mirror upstairs,」 he said.

He brought a cheval glass down to the living room, and I had him stand with his chest against the side of it, so that his troublesome left arm was behind it and his normal right arm was in front. He tipped his head so that when he looked into the mirror the image of his right arm seemed to occupy the same position as his left arm. Then I had him wave his arms, his actual arms, as if he were conducting an orchestra.

The first thing he expressed was disappointment. 「It isn』t quite like looking at my left hand,」 he said. But then suddenly it was.

「Wow!」 he said. 「Now, this is odd.」

After a moment or two, I noticed that he had stopped moving his left arm. Yet he reported that he still felt as if it were moving. What』s more, the sensations in it had changed dramatically. For the first time in eleven years, he felt his left hand 「snap」 back to normal size. He felt the burning pain in his arm diminish. And the itch, too, was dulled.

「This is positively bizarre,」 he said.

He still felt the pain and the itch in his neck and shoulder, where the image in the mirror cut off. And, when he came away from the mirror, the aberrant sensations in his left arm returned. He began using the mirror a few times a day, for fifteen minutes or so at a stretch, and I checked in with him periodically.

「What』s most dramatic is the change in the size of my hand,」 he says. After a couple of weeks, his hand returned to feeling normal in size all day long.

The mirror also provided the first effective treatment he has had for the flares of itch and pain that sporadically seize him. Where once he could do nothing but sit and wait for the torment to subside—it sometimes took an hour or more—he now just pulls out the mirror. 「I』ve never had anything like this before,」 he said. 「It』s my magic mirror.」

There have been other, isolated successes with mirror treatment. In Bath, England, several patients suffering from what is called complex regional pain syndrome—severe, disabling limb sensations of unknown cause—were reported to have experienced complete resolution after six weeks of mirror therapy. In California, mirror therapy helped stroke patients recover from a condition known as hemineglect, which produces something like the opposite of a phantom limb—these patients have a part of the body they no longer realize is theirs.

Such findings open up a fascinating prospect: perhaps many patients whom doctors treat as having a nerve injury or a disease have, instead, what might be called sensor syndromes. When your car』s dashboard warning light keeps telling you that there is an engine failure, but the mechanics can』t find anything wrong, the sensor itself may be the problem. This is no less true for human beings. Our sensations of pain, itch, nausea, and fatigue are normally protective. Unmoored from physical reality, however, they can become a nightmare: M., with her intractable itching, and H., with his constellation of strange symptoms—but perhaps also the hundreds of thousands of people in the United States alone who suffer from conditions like chronic back pain, fibromyalgia, chronic pelvic pain, tinnitus, temporomandibular joint disorder, or repetitive strain injury, where, typically, no amount of imaging, nerve testing, or surgery manages to uncover an anatomical explanation. Doctors have persisted in treating these conditions as nerve or tissue problems—engine failures, as it were. We get under the hood and remove this, replace that, snip some wires. Yet still the sensor keeps going off.

So we get frustrated. 「There』s nothing wrong,」 we』ll insist. And, the next thing you know, we』re treating the driver instead of the problem. We prescribe tranquillizers, antidepressants, escalating doses of narcotics. And the drugs often do make it easier for people to ignore the sensors, even if they are wired right into the brain. The mirror treatment, by contrast, targets the deranged sensor system itself. It essentially takes a misfiring sensor—a warning system functioning under an illusion that something is terribly wrong out in the world it monitors—and feeds it an alternate set of signals that calm it down. The new signals may even reset the sensor.

This may help explain, for example, the success of the advice that back specialists now commonly give. Work through the pain, they tell many of their patients, and, surprisingly often, the pain goes away. It had been a mystifying phenomenon. But the picture now seems clearer. Most chronic back pain starts as an acute back pain—say, after a fall. Usually, the pain subsides as the injury heals. But in some cases the pain sensors continue to light up long after the tissue damage is gone. In such instances, working through the pain may offer the brain contradictory feedback—a signal that ordinary activity does not, in fact, cause physical harm. And so the sensor resets.

This understanding of sensation points to an entire new array of potential treatments—based not on drugs or surgery but, instead, on the careful manipulation of our perceptions. Researchers at the University of Manchester, in England, have gone a step beyond mirrors and fashioned an immersive virtual-reality system for treating patients with phantom-limb pain. Detectors transpose movement of real limbs into a virtual world where patients feel they are actually moving, stretching, even playing a ballgame. So far, five patients have tried the system, and they have all experienced a reduction in pain. Whether those results will last has yet to be established. But the approach raises the possibility of designing similar systems to help patients with other sensor syndromes. How, one wonders, would someone with chronic back pain fare in a virtual world? The Manchester study suggests that there may be many ways to fight our phantoms.

I called Ramachandran to ask him about M.』s terrible itch. The sensation may be a phantom, but it』s on her scalp, not in a limb, so it seemed unlikely that his mirror approach could do anything for her. He told me about an experiment in which he put ice-cold water in people』s ears. This confuses the brain』s position sensors, tricking subjects into thinking that their heads are moving, and in certain phantom-limb and stroke patients the illusion corrected their misperceptions, at least temporarily. Maybe this would help M., he said. He had another idea. If you take two mirrors and put them at right angles to each other, you will get a non-reversed mirror image. Looking in, the right half of your face appears on the left and the left half appears on the right. But unless you move, he said, your brain may not realize that the image is flipped.

「Now, suppose she looks in this mirror and scratches the left side of her head. No, wait—I』m thinking out loud here—suppose she looks and you have someone elsetouch the left side of her head. It』ll look—maybe it』ll feel—like you』re touching the right side of her head.」 He let out an impish giggle. 「Maybe this would make her itchy right scalp feel more normal.」 Maybe it would encourage her brain to make a different perceptual inference; maybe it would press reset. 「Who knows?」 he said.

It seemed worth a try. ?


噗,不知道生理上為什麼會有癢。但是這幫助我們理解了心理上的「癢」是怎麼回事。


有深度的問題,我就用我淺薄的知識來回答一下吧。

首先我認為「癢」和」撓」之間並不是一種因果關係,不同於」痛」與」縮手」之間的關係,後者的關係是一種條件反射,是無意識的自主神經的反射性活動,就像肚子餓的時候看到美食就會自動分泌唾液一樣。而癢的時候去撓是一種有意識的神經活動,因為癢的時候你可以選擇撓,也可以選擇不撓,也可以選擇拍打、針刺等手段來克服癢的感覺。

我在臨床上見過陰囊濕疹的病人,癢的時候拚命撓,結果越撓越癢,越撓越舒服,撓的陰囊皮膚破潰、出血、糜爛,還在撓,這是一種惡性循環,因此我臨床上多建議病人以吃藥的方式克服癢,西藥如抗組胺葯氯雷他定、西替利嗪等,或通過中藥清熱祛濕等方法一樣可以達到止癢的目的。如果是身體其他部位,可以通過輕輕拍打的方式止癢,因為輕微的疼痛就可以抑制癢的感覺。


或許是皮膚表面的細菌和真菌製造皮膚的瘙癢,促使人去撓破皮膚,從而細菌真菌們能在溫暖又舒適的人體內繁衍生息


個人認為「癢是一種慾望」,相較疼痛等無意識感覺,癢是一種有意識的感覺,意識會驅動自己做出一些反應去回應這個慾望,比如七年之癢。。。


脂肪分子的不規則運動,造成的正常效果。


弗洛伊德認為,癢就是一種力比多。力比多就是性愛的信號了。現在的很多書刊都在告訴你,男人需要在女人的哪些部位進行調情,觸發女人哪些部位容易激發對方性高潮。其實無非就是陰部、乳房、大腿、頸部、耳部、腋下、肋骨、腳掌等部位。而這些部位也是最容易發癢最敏感的部位。

在人類還沒有進入一夫一妻制的漫長的採獵時期,人們過著雜交的生活,只知其母不知其父,由此人類只能靠母系來維持社會關係,所以只能是母系社會。這個階段,男性只享受性交的快感,但性交後懷孕和撫育孩子的重任卻落在了女人身上。遠古人類採取雜交的繁殖方式使得女性懷孕的機會很大,一生大部分時間都在懷孕和撫育孩子,再加上女性本身體力就不如男性,這樣就無法勝任打獵的工作,生活難以得到保障,也無法使自己和孩子得到安全保護。於是,女人「預謀」了一場「變革」,為使自己得到更多男人的青睞,幫助她們撫育孩子、採集食物以及分享獵物。女人的詭計中有兩點跟癢相關:

第一,褪毛。褪毛以後,肌膚暴露出來,在這種蛻變過程中,女性的皮膚也越來越精緻,增加了美感。而且,褪毛以後才能露出自己的痒痒肉,按照弗洛伊德理論,「癢」就是一種力比多,即性慾。女人褪毛以後對性變得更加敏感,大大增加了男性性生活的樂趣,使得男人更加愛護女人。


第二,性高潮。人類女性性高潮時候的興奮、抽搐、痙攣、幻想,在整個生物界是不可思議的事情,無疑,這又是女人的詭計,為了取悅男人,獲得關照。


這個進化論的說法有待商榷,畢竟人類不能完全按照自己的意志進化自己,但也並不能排除這種可能。

我覺得癢這種感覺跟性是有很大關係的,但個人直覺,癢並不一定就是性。癢這種感覺只有在性幻想的配合下才能成為一種性慾。平時鬧著玩,有朋友撓我肋骨,我的JJ並不會大起來(好害羞的說),但如果是做愛的時候女方撫摸我的肋骨,那。。。你懂的。自古有撓痒痒的刑罰,有不堪其癢致死的,我覺得這種癢並沒有激發任何性衝動。所以,人是一種非常複雜的動物,同樣是癢,有性幻想的情況下就很爽,沒有的時候就不那麼舒服,真是匪夷所思。人類對自身的研究還差很遠很遠,很多精細的東西都難以理解。比如同性戀,異性相吸是自然規律,但為什麼有的人偏偏違逆自然規律異性不再相吸呢?這絕對不是單純的心理原因吧?


——————————————————————————


補充:


癢這種感覺據說是人體的一種過敏反應,在過敏時候會分泌一種蛋白質——組胺,組胺會支配一些發癢神經向脊髓傳遞信息,信息在脊髓得到處理後,反饋給大腦。


科學家還發現,激發癢的感覺的化學物質並非一種,還有神經纖維,而且還不只是一種神經纖維,對到底什麼物質激發了人類癢的感覺目前科學界尚在探索之中。但進化論認為,人類會有癢的感覺,是因為這種不自主的迅速反應動作是人類在遠古時代逃避動物或昆蟲侵犯時的一種本能。


更多閱讀人為什麼會癢


癢就是輕微的疼痛,沒別的。


我覺得不光癢是多餘的,這個世上有太多東西多餘了。但不管任何存在都有其淵源,有的科學家至今無法解釋。比如你的問題其實應該是[為什麼要有多餘的感覺]?多餘這種東西從來都是因人而異,你覺得自己多餘嗎?你以為自己為社會創造了價值,不是多餘的,換位去想,如果地球有感覺的話,它只會認為又多了一個毫無意義而且只會浪費自己身上資源,你這個多餘的存在。


我曾經採訪過1982年諾貝爾生理或醫學獎得主本格特·薩米爾松,詢問過這個問題,老先生現在就在研究致癢原理這個方向,他希望自己可以得到答案。應該很難的~~~


推薦閱讀:

天生蠻力是不是真的?為什麼?

TAG:進化 | 生活常識 | 神經學 | 身體 | 身體機能 |